Language Teaching Research 13 ,4 (2009); pp. 425–449
Examining
the effectiveness of explicit instruction of vocabulary learning strategies
with Japanese EFL university students
Atsushi
Mizumoto University of Marketing and Distribution
Sciences and
Osamu
Takeuchi Kansai University
This study examined the effectiveness of explicit
instruction of vocabulary learning strategies (VLSs) over a 10-week semester
with a group of 146 female EFL learners from two Japanese universities. A
vocabulary test and questionnaires on VLSs and motivation were administered at
the beginning of the course. The learners were divided into two groups based on
the vocabulary test results: an experimental group and a control group. Only
the experimental group received explicit instruction on VLSs in combination
with their regular language lessons. The same instruments were r e-administered
at the end of the course to examine the changes in both the questionnaire
responses and test scores. Qualitative analyses were also conducted to explore
the findings in detail. The results show that the experimental group
outperformed the control group in the vocabulary test. It was also found that
(1) strategy training was effective for both changing the repertoire of
strategies used and improving their frequency of use, (2) the training
increased the use of certain strategies more than it did for other strategies,
and (3) different types of learners exhibited different responses to the
strategy instruction. This study’s findings contribute to a better
understanding of strategy instruction in general and VLSs in particular.
Keywords: vocabulary
learning strategies, strategy instruction, explicit teaching, learner
strategies
I Introduction
Since the onset of learning
strategy research some three decades ago (for a comprehensive review, see Cohen
and Macaro, 2007), the promise of intervention studies, i.e. teaching students
learning strategies, has been widely r ecognized. Rubin (1975) emphasized that
‘the inclusion of knowledge about the good language learner in our classroom
instructional strategies will lessen the difference between the good learner
and the poorer one’ (p. 50). Furthermore, a recent review by Rubin et al. (2007) with regard to
Address for correspondence:
Atsushi Mizumoto, 2-23-33, Ohmiya, Asahi-ku, Osaka, 535-0002, Japan; email:
atsushi@mizumot.com
© The Author(s), 2009. Reprints and
permissions: 10.1177/1362168809341511
http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journalspermissions.nav
i ntervention studies relating to language learning strategies
suggests that teaching students learning strategies, if effectively done,
increases not only their knowledge of strategies but also their motivation and
performance. With these notions, a wealth of research on the effectiveness of
learning strategies instruction has been conducted to date. Although some
researchers such as Dörnyei (2005) point out that ‘the currently available
evidence gives only moderate support, at best, for strategy training’ (p. 177),
the general consensus in the field is that learning strategies instruction
warrants time and effort both in and out of the classroom (see, among others,
Oxford, 1990; Cohen, 1998; Chamot et al.,
1999; Macaro, 2001; Ikeda, 2007). Given the high teachability of learning
strategies, it is natural that practitioners would attempt to teach the
strategies used by the more successful learners to the less successful ones,
thereby facilitating or modifying their learning process.
Among the
several types of learning strategies (Cohen and Macaro, 2007), vocabulary
learning strategies (VLSs) have attracted the attention of many researchers
around the world (Ahmed, 1989; Sanaoui, 1995; Gu and Johnson, 1996; Lawson and
Hogben, 1996; Schmitt, 1997; Kojic-Sabo and Lightbown, 1999; Nakamura, 2002;
Catalán, 2003; Fan, 2003; Gu, 2003a). This is because mastering vocabulary is
one of the most challenging tasks that any learner faces while acquiring
another language and, thus, learners have consistently found it necessary to
compensate for their limited vocabulary (Nyikos and Fan, 2007). This situation
is especially true for EFL environments, for instance, in Japan, where exposure
to English in daily life is extremely limited; consequently, vocabulary
acquisition does not come naturally. Thus, the importance of VLSs has been
emphasized along with the strategies related to the four other skills required
in an EFL environment, namely, listening, speaking, reading, and writing (e.g.
Takeuchi, 2003).
The importance
of VLSs has been reflected in the fact that intervention studies have been
conducted in addition to descriptive studies. Intervention studies relating to
VLSs began focusing on memory strategies ( commonly known as mnemonics) in the
early 1980s (Meara, 1980; Cohen and Aphek, 1981), and a large portion of the
past research on VLSs i nstruction was
conducted during this time. Research on memory strategies instruction
has been mostly propelled by the depth-of-processing theory (Craik and Lockhart,
1972) in which ‘deeper’ processing is construed as being superior to ‘shallow’
processing. Subsequently, similar studies have been carried out using more
sophisticated methods (e.g. Atay and Ozbuigan, 2007). Although some researchers
(e.g. Gu, 2003b) point out the limitations of mnemonics, the empirical findings
suggest that memory strategies are effective for retaining vocabulary,
especially when used in combination with other strategies ( Brown and
Perry, 1991).
While previous
studies on memory strategies instruction have generally reported positive
results, one study by O’Malley et al.
(1985) presented inconsistent findings. They conducted a study in a classroom
setting to i nvestigate whether instruction on using a combination of
various strategies – incorporating metacognitive, cognitive, and
social/affective strategies – would result in improved learning with respect to
speaking, listening, and vocabulary tasks. The result of the vocabulary test
revealed that there were no differences among the treatment groups. However,
when the groups were divided according to ethnicity groups, it was discovered
that the Asian control group outperformed the Asian experimental group, thus
indicating that the Asian learners could not take advantage of the strategies
that were taught (in this case, self-evaluation for metacognitive strategies;
imagery and grouping for cognitive strategies). They also concluded that the
Asian learners preferred to use the learning strategy of rote repetition. This
study indicated the possibility that cognitively demanding VLSs – namely,
imagery and grouping – do not necessarily work for all types of learners.
Therefore, it is important in VLSs instruction to consider individual
differences in terms of, for example, motivation level, gender, self-efficacy,
career orientation, proficiency, and the learning environment (context) in
which the learners are situated (Oxford and Nyikos, 1989; Cohen and Dörnyei,
2002; Gu, 2003b; Takeuchi et al.,
2007).
Another
important point in VLSs instruction is the inclusion of metacognitive
strategies. Rasekh and Ranjbary (2003) examined the effect of a 10-week,
explicit metacognitive strategy training session conducted in a classroom
setting with Iranian EFL students. They reported a positive effect on vocabulary
learning. Zaki and Ellis (1999) also demonstrated that teaching metacognitive
strategies brings about better vocabulary learning. The use of meta cognitive
strategies is also know as the ‘structured approach’ (Sanaoui, 1995), and its
importance is emphasized by Folse (2004) as follows: ‘It does not seem to
matter so much what students do with new vocabulary provided that they do
something and that they do it consistently’ (p. 91). Therefore, it appears that
VLSs instruction including metacognitive strategies would prove to be more
beneficial than instruction without such strategies.
Although a
number of studies on VLSs instruction have contributed to a better
understanding of its effectiveness as mentioned above, such studies have a few
limitations. First, the studies on memory strategies have focused on isolated
strategies such as the keyword method. However, as O’Malley and Chamot (1990)
emphasized, our understanding of VLSs instruction might benefit from examining
‘a training system in which multiple strategies are taught within a single
package’ (p. 169). Additionally, considering the aforementioned importance of
metacognitive strategies in VLSs instruction, they should be taught in
combination with other cognitive strategies.
Second, the
instruction period in past VLSs research tended to vary greatly from study to
study. The majority of researchers favoring strategy instruction propose that
it can be most successful when incorporated into the regular classroom
instruction (McDonough, 1999). If we aim to incorporate VLSs in this manner,
the instruction period should span more than a few lessons. No study, however,
has addressed this issue thus far. Finally, no previous study has investigated
learners’ initial repertoire of VLSs and how such instruction has caused them
to change their strategies.
Addressing the shortcomings of the past
studies on VLSs instruction, the present study aims to answer the following
research questions by providing explicit instruction on VLSs in combination
with the regular classroom instruction for 10 weeks to Japanese EFL university
students.
•
Were there any differences between the experimental
group and control group after 10 weeks of VLSs instruction?
•
If any, in which strategies did the differences emerge?
•
Were there any differences within the experimental
group that were based on the learners’ initial repertoire of VLSs?
•
If so, what are the reasons behind these differences?
II Method
The experimental group
received VLSs instruction during the 10-week course; however, the control group
only received the regular classroom instruction. In order to answer the first
research question, we administered questionnaires to both the groups before and
after the course. We then compared the results of the two groups. For the
second research question, we focused on the experimental group. First, using a
cluster analysis, the experimental group was divided according to the learners’
initial repertoire of VLSs. Next, the pretest and posttest scores were compared
within the experimental group. The alpha for all statistical decisions was set
at .05. Finally, for the third research question, we once again focused on the
experimental group and investigated the qualitative data collected from the
participants in the group.
1 Participants and instruments
A total of 204 female EFL learners from two private
universities (116 and 88 learners, respectively) in western Japan participated
in the current study. Their ages ranged from 18 (first year) to 22 (fourth
year), and all were majoring in humanities. Since the participants were divided
into an experimental group and a control group based on their institutions,
random assignment of the participants was unfeasible. Therefore, we used a
vocabulary test developed by Mizumoto and Shimamoto (2008) to form the two
groups. The rationale behind the use of this vocabulary test is that lexical
competence is a crucial factor in almost all the aspects of L2 proficiency (e.g.
Zareva et al., 2005). This is
especially true for an EFL setting, where exposure to English in everyday life
is either very limited or nonexistent. Therefore, the test would enable us to
obtain not only the learners’ vocabulary knowledge, but also an overall picture
of their proficiency levels. In addition, we chose this vocabulary test for the
current study because Aizawa (1998) pointed out several problems (e.g. the
difficulty of understanding definitions or the high ratio of loan words in
Japanese) related to using the Vocabulary Levels Test (Nation, 1990) – the most
widely-used standardized vocabulary measurement test – for Japanese EFL
learners.
The vocabulary
test used in the current study originally had 160 items, each with four
multiple-choice options. The target words were selected from the JACET 8000
word list (JACET, 2003), which was compiled specifically for Japanese EFL
learners; this is another reason why we considered this instrument to be more
appropriate for the current study. Of the 160 items, 25 items that contained
the target words that were taught during the course were selected and included
in the analyses. The same test was re-administered at the end of the course to
measure the improvements in vocabulary knowledge.
Based on the
results of the pre vocabulary test, a matching procedure (Dörnyei, 2007, p.
118), which identifies participants with the same or similar test scores in the
two comparison groups, was used to divide the participants into two groups: an
experimental group (n = 76) and a
control group (n = 70). The mean
scores of the vocabulary test were 15.80 (SD
= 2.91) for the experimental group and 15.41 (SD = 2.99) for the control group. A twotailed independent t-test confirmed that a statistically
significant difference did not exist between the results of the two groups (t = 0.79, df = 144, p = .43, r = .07). Therefore, they were
considered to be equal in terms of vocabulary knowledge.
The program in
which they were enrolled was a TOEIC (Test of English for International Communication)
test preparation course, and both the experimental and taught identical content
with identical materials. The study took place between September 2006 and
January 2007 for approximately four months, which is the typical period spanned
by one semester in Japanese universities. The participants’ proficiency was
measured by their TOEIC IP (Institutional Program)1 scores
(experimental group: n = 69, M = 436.38, SD = 105.16; control group: n
= 54, M = 429.38, SD = 118.09).2 According to
the TOEIC Steering Committee (2006), the mean TOEIC scores for university
students majoring in humanities and engineering are 474 and 397, respectively.
Therefore, the participants of this study can be regarded as average or
lower-level university EFL students.
A VLSs questionnaire
was administered to all the participants at both the beginning and end of the
course. This questionnaire was developed to measure learners’ intentional
vocabulary learning behaviors while they are in the process of memorizing new
vocabulary (cognitive strategies) and coordinating their strategic behaviors
(metacognitive strategies). It was confirmed in a previous study (Mizumoto and
Takeuchi, 2008) that the questionnaire could function as a psychometrically
valid scale. The substrategies (subscales) of the overall intentional
vocabulary consolidation learning strategies include the following: (1)
Self-management, (2) Input-seeking, (3) Imagery,
(4) Writing rehearsal, (5) Oral rehearsal, and (6)
Association. These c ategories were established using Rasch analysis and
exploratory/confirmatory factor analysis (Mizumoto and Takeuchi, 2009) and were
named based on a previous line of studies (e.g. Gu and Johnson, 1996; Schmitt,
1997; Fan, 2003).
In addition to
the VLSs questionnaire, as a measure of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation,
nine items from the questionnaire developed by Noels et al. (2000)3 were utilized. Motivation measures were
included primarily because, in related literature, motivation is considered to
have the strongest influence on the choice of learning strategies (Oxford and
Nyikos, 1989).
Both of the questionnaires used a
5-point Likert scale, with 1 indicating ‘not at all true of me’ and 5
indicating ‘very true of me.’ The items of each questionnaire are presented in
Appendixes 1 and 2. The score for each substrategy was calculated by averaging
each item’s score. Since we were using a questionnaire established in a
previous study, the construct validity of the questionnaires was investigated
through a confirmatory factor analysis (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2006). According
to the result, the model for VLSs showed an acceptable fit to the data (χ2 = 419.55, df = 260, CFI = .906, GFI =
.817, RMSEA = .065).4 The
model for motivation was also within the acceptable range (χ2 = 69.19, df = 26, CFI = .964, GFI = .934, RMSEA = .085). All the analyses in this study were conducted using SPSS
14.0 and AMOS 5.0. Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the pretest
instruments.
2 Qualitative data collection procedures
Some researchers (e.g. Denzin,
1997; Dörnyei, 2007) recommend method triangulation wherein the findings are
based on not only a primary source but also a secondary source of information.
Following this recommendation, we incorporated qualitative analyses into this
study in order to clarify the causes
Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the pretest instruments (n = 146)
|
Number of
Possible M
items range
|
SD
|
Skewness
|
Kurtosis
|
a
|
||
Self-management
( VLSs )
|
7
|
1–5
|
2.57
|
0.75
|
0.46
|
0.53
|
.80
|
Input-seeking
( VLSs )
|
4
|
1–5
|
3.16
|
0.94
|
–0.03
|
–0.50
|
.87
|
Imagery (VLSs)
|
5
|
1–5
|
3.08
|
0.80
|
0.15
|
0.16
|
.79
|
Writing
rehearsal
( VLSs )
|
3
|
1–5
|
3.72
|
0.92
|
–0.31
|
–0.52
|
.77
|
Oral rehearsal (VLSs)
|
3
|
1–5
|
3.17
|
1.00
|
0.19
|
–0.63
|
.87
|
Association (VLSs)
|
3
|
1–5
|
2.55
|
0.88
|
0.13
|
–0.04
|
.88
|
Extrinsic motivation
|
3
|
1–5
|
3.53
|
0.89
|
–0.15
|
–0.57
|
.74
|
Intrinsic motivation
|
6
|
1–5
|
3.50
|
0.87
|
–0.21
|
–0.46
|
.87
|
Vocabulary test
|
25
|
0–25
|
15.62
|
2.95
|
–0.33
|
–0.10
|
.66
|
of the findings obtained
through the quantitative data sources, namely, the questionnaires and
vocabulary test. Specifically, the learners in the experimental group were
asked to maintain study logs throughout the course, and interview sessions were
conducted with them at the end of the course. The participants were directed to
record the details of their daily study of English and the VLSs and materials
that they used each day. In addition, follow-up interview sessions were
conducted to determine the strategies that the participants employed outside
the classroom during the course. A total of nine individuals, who were randomly
chosen from among the participants in the experimental group, were interviewed.
Participation in the interview sessions was voluntary. Furthermore, since the
interview sessions took place outside of the regular class time, the
participants were presented with a bookstore gift certificate (valued at
¥1000). At least two students were interviewed in each session and, due to the
semi-structured nature of the interviews, the participants were able to
exchange ideas on how they felt about the various strategies. All the
interviews were conducted in Japanese, and each session lasted approximately 45
minutes (about 20 minutes per individual). The questionnaires and study logs
submitted by the participants were provided at each interview session to help
the participants recall what they had done during the course. With the
participants’ consent, the interviews were recorded with an IC recorder and
subsequently transcribed by one of the authors of this article.
3 Investigation of the initial repertoire of VLSs
After the administration of the VLSs and motivation
questionnaires and the vocabulary test at the beginning of the course, the
participants in the experimental group were grouped using a cluster analysis,
which was based on their initial repertoire of VLSs. The Ward method with the
squared Euclidean distance technique was used in applying the cluster analysis.
This particular method was chosen because the combination has been known to
‘combine clusters with a small number of observations, and produce clusters
with approximately the same number of observations’, (Hiromori, 2006) and,
thus, it is highly suitable for revealing individual differences (Yamamori et al., 2003).
Figure 1
illustrates the cluster profiles of the three groups, and Table 2 presents the
scores, standard deviations, and results of the post hoc tests (Tukey’s multiple comparison technique). All the
scores of the variables investigated (those in Table 1) were first transformed
into z-scores and then entered into
the cluster analysis. This is because the unit of measurement for the
vocabulary test results was different from that of the questionnaires. Based on
a dendrogram, which is a tree-like graphic display of the distances between
each combining cluster, it was decided that the participants could be divided
into three groups. This decision was supported by a one-way ANOVA, which
revealed statistically significant differences among the three groups (p < .05).
management
seeking rehearsal rehearsal motivation motivation
Figure 1 Cluster profiles of VLSs among the three groups before treatment
Table 2 Descriptions of each cluster (SD is given in brackets)
|
Cluster 1
(n
= 36)
|
Cluster 2
(n
= 14)
|
Cluster 3
(n
= 26)
|
Significant in post hoc test
|
Self-management
|
2.59 (0.63)
|
3.48 (0.82)
|
2.43 (0.58)
|
Clusters 1–2, 2–3
|
Input-seeking
|
2.47 (0.74)
|
4.04 (0.61)
|
3.53 (0.77)
|
Clusters 1–2, 1–3
|
Imagery
|
2.91 (0.73)
|
4.13 (0.64)
|
2.94 (0.54)
|
Clusters 1–2, 2–3
|
Writing rehearsal
|
3.78 (0.79)
|
3.91 (1.06)
|
4.21 (0.78)
|
–
|
Oral rehearsal
|
2.72 (0.85)
|
3.86 (1.04)
|
3.27 (0.78)
|
Clusters 1–2, 1–3
|
Association
|
2.30 (0.78)
|
3.74 (0.72)
|
2.60 (0.57)
|
Clusters 1–2, 2–3
|
Extrinsic motivation
|
3.38 (0.77)
|
3.86 (0.79)
|
3.88 (0.86)
|
Clusters 1–3
|
Intrinsic motivation
|
3.08 (0.77)
|
4.14 (0.63)
|
3.86 (0.93)
|
Clusters 1–2, 1–3
|
Vocabulary test
|
14.42 (2.16)
|
16.43 (3.78)
|
17.38 (2.43)
|
Clusters 1–2, 1–3
|
Note : For all significant pairs in post
hoc test: p < .05
The learners belonging to Cluster 1 can
be referred to as the ‘less frequent strategy users’. Compared with the other
two groups, their overall use of strategies was lower, and they mostly relied
on writing rehearsal strategies. On the other hand, the learners in Cluster 2
were ‘active strategy users’. They reported coordinating several strategies
more often than did the learners in
Clusters 1 and 3. They used the self-management and input-seeking s trategies –
which are metacognitive strategies – more frequently, and they had the highest
intrinsic motivation of the three groups. Those in Cluster 3 were ‘moderate
strategy users’. As can be seen in Figure 1, the profile of their s trategy use
lies just between those of Clusters 1 and 2. From these results, it was assumed
that the three groups were different in terms of their strategy use at the
beginning of the course.
4 Instruction of vocabulary learning strategies
In the experimental group, students whose strategy use
differed were instructed to sit near each other in the classroom so that they
could interact and exchange their ideas and opinions about the strategies being
taught during the training session. This type of interaction was included to
help promote scaffolding in the Zone of Proximal Development (e.g. van Lier,
2004) wherein learners with different types of strategies can help each other
by sharing how they approach the task at hand.
Both the experimental and control groups
attended one class per week (90 minutes). For the experimental group, the set
of the cognitive and metacognitive strategies presented in Table 3 was taught
explicitly during the regular class, with each VLSs instruction lasting
approximately 30 minutes. The instruction took place in the first 30 minutes of
a 90-minute class.
Table 3 Schedule of VLSs instruction
Week
|
Cognitive strategies
|
M etacognitive strategies
|
|
1
|
Pre-questionnaire and vocabulary test
|
|
|
2
|
Vocalization of the words, phrases, and sentences
|
+
|
Conscious preview
|
3
|
Use of collocations or phrases
|
+
|
Start
of vocabulary learning with a preview
|
4
|
Imagery strategies
|
+
|
Expansion of one’s own way of learning
vocabulary
|
5
|
Writing and oral rehearsal
|
+
|
Conscious input of English vocabulary
|
6
|
Grouping of semanticallyrelated words
|
+
|
Target-setting in vocabulary
learning
|
7
|
Mnemonics
(keyword methods)
|
+
|
Aim
of designating time for vocabulary learning
|
8
|
Association of the target words with familiar synonyms or antonyms
|
+
|
Attempts to actually use new
words while learning
new vocabulary
|
9
|
Use of
prefixes and suffixes
( or stems )
|
+
|
Testing vocabulary regularly
|
10
|
Effective use of vocabulary notes or cards
|
+
|
Goal
to remember a certain number of words
|
11
|
Review & post-questionnaire and vocabulary test
|
|
|
Note : The
students were instructed in both cognitive and metacognitive strategies.
On the other hand, the control group spent the same amount
of time on other activities such as reviewing the previously taught contents
not related to vocabulary learning.
The target
strategies were chosen based on the findings of previous VLSs research (e.g. Gu
and Johnson, 1996; Schmitt, 1997; Fan, 2003) and on the vocabulary that was to
be taught in each lesson. With regard to the instruction method, we chose
explicit strategy instruction because the existing strategy instruction models
place much emphasis on its importance (Oxford, 1990; Wenden, 1991; Cohen, 1998;
Chamot et al., 1999). Among the
various strategy training frameworks (Dörnyei, 2005, p. 174), we based our
method of instruction on the model proposed by Chamot et al. (1999) due to its simplicity and the ease with which
learners can follow it.5 The order of instruction was as follows:
(1) preparation, (2) presentation, (3) practice, (4) expansion, and (5)
evaluation.
In each
lesson, the instructor prepared a handout containing certain target words taken
from the course textbook and corresponding example sentences. Gu (2003b) states
that ‘the choice, use, and effectiveness of VLSs depend on the task, the
learner, and the learning context.’ Correspondingly, the participants’ task in
this study was to learn the target words by using the strategies introduced in
each lesson. In addition to the handout, a slideshow introducing and explaining
the effectiveness of each vocabulary learning strategy was shown during the
presentation phase.
During the practice stage, the
instructor explicitly introduced and demonstrated the target strategies, and
the students applied them to learning vocabulary while discussing their use
with classmates. They also had an opportunity to discuss whether they used such
strategies often while they learned and how they felt about the target
strategies or the possible applications of the strategies being taught.
Following the practice stage, as an initial evaluation, the participants were
asked to answer the following questions according to a five-point scale:
1)
whether they were already using the strategies;
2)
how useful they felt the strategies were;
3)
how suitable they thought the strategies were to them;
and 4) whether they felt that they would like to try using the strategies.
For the
expansion and the second evaluation phases, the learners applied the strategies
introduced for an assignment and recorded their evaluation of the strategy’s
usefulness in the study logs. They were directed to experiment with the
introduced strategy and then report in the study log whether or not they
thought that the VLS would suit them. For the assignments, they were allowed to
use other VLSs in addition to the one introduced in the previous lesson.
However, the use of the target VLS was mandatory. In the following lesson, the
students were required to submit their study logs and take a review quiz of the
target vocabulary.
With respect to the control group,
another instructor conducted regular classes with the same materials as the
ones used by the experimental group. Thus, the learners also received handouts
containing target words from the course textbook and corresponding example
sentences; however, they were not required to maintain a study log. Moreover,
caution was taken to ensure that the control group was not taught anything
related to VLSs during the l essons. Thus, the differences in treatment between
the experimental and c ontrol groups were whether or not (1) the participants
were required to maintain a study log, and (2) they received VLSs instruction
in the classroom.
III Results and discussion
1 Were there any differences between the experimental and control groups after 10 weeks of VLSs instruction? If any, in which strategies did the differences emerge?
Table 4 summarizes the scores and corresponding standard
deviations for all the variables investigated before and after the course for
both the groups and the gains with regard to their scores after the treatment.
The gains in the scores were tested with the Bonferroni adjustment to control
for Type I error inflation.
In order to examine whether there were
significant differences with respect to the VLSs questionnaire or the
vocabulary test based on the intervention, a 2 (Group) × 2 (Time) repeated
measures multivariate analysis
Table 4 Scores, standard deviations, and gains in the two groups (SD is given
in brackets)
Pretest
|
Posttest
|
Gain
|
Pretest
|
Posttest
|
Gain
|
|
Self-management
|
2.70 (0.75)
|
2.85 (0.79)
|
0.15
|
2.42 (0.74)
|
2.54 (0.80)
|
0.12
|
Input-seeking
|
3.12 (0.97)
|
3.41 (0.89)
|
0.29*
|
3.21 (0.92)
|
3.24 (0.99)
|
0.03
|
Imagery
|
3.14 (0.80)
|
3.23 (0.94)
|
0.09
|
3.02 (0.80)
|
3.20 (0.82)
|
0.18
|
Writing rehearsal
|
3.95 (0.85)
|
3.91 (0.99)
|
–0.04
|
3.46 (0.93)
|
3.60 (1.10)
|
0.13
|
Oral rehearsal
|
3.12 (0.96)
|
3.60 (0.90)
|
0.48*
|
3.22 (1.05)
|
3.19 (1.04)
|
–0.03
|
Association
|
2.67 (0.87)
|
3.00 (0.94)
|
0.33*
|
2.41 (0.87)
|
2.63 (0.88)
|
0.21
|
Extrinsic motivation
|
3.64 (0.83)
|
3.53 (0.93)
|
–0.11
|
3.40 (0.94)
|
3.41 (1.00)
|
0.01
|
Intrinsic motivation
|
3.54 (0.92)
|
3.68 (0.86)
|
0.14
|
3.51 (0.82)
|
3.58 (0.95)
|
0.07
|
Vocabulary test
|
15.80 (2.91)
|
18.42 (3.86)
|
2.62* 15.41 (2.99)
16.10 (2.96)
|
0.69
|
*
*
Note : Gain is the mean difference (posttest minus
pretest); *p < .05 with the
Bonferroni adjustment.
of variance (MANOVA) was conducted. To use MANOVA, the
following three c onditions must be satisfied (Weinfurt, 1995, p. 253): (1)
multivariate n ormality, (2) homogeneity of the covariance matrices, and (3)
independence of observations. Therefore, prior to carrying out the MANOVA, we
confirmed that the data met these conditions.
The result of
the MANOVA revealed a significant multivariate main effect for both Group
(Wilks’ Λ = .85, F(9, 136) = 2.73, p < 05, h2 = .15) and
Time (Wilks’ Λ = .67, F(9, 136) = 7.54, p < .05, h2 = .33). More
importantly, there was a significant Group × Time interaction (Wilks’ Λ = .80, F(9, 136) = 3.75, p <
05, h2
= .20). This interaction effect indicates that a difference does exist between
the experimental and control groups based on the linear combination of all the
dependent variables investigated.
Follow-up
univariate repeated ANOVAs for each dependent variable revealed that the main
effect of time (gain before and after the treatment) was significant for all
the variables, with the exception of Writing Rehearsal and Extrinsic
Motivation. The most important finding in this particular analysis was that the
interaction between group and time was significant for Input-seeking (F(1, 144) = 4.98, p < .05, partial h2 = .03) and
Oral rehearsal (F(1, 144) = 11.69, p < .05, partial h2 =
.08). This interaction effect ( illustrated in Figure 2) strongly suggests that
the change in these two dependent v ariables over time is associated with the
intervention, namely, the instruction of VLSs. In addition, the interaction
between group and time was significant for the vocabulary test (F(1, 144) = 14.40, p < .05, partial h2 = .09),
indicating that the experimental group outperformed the control group after
receiving VLSs instruction.
From these results, it can be concluded
that the instruction of VLSs produced the difference in results between the
experimental and control groups, specifically with respect to the input-seeking
and oral rehearsal strategies and the vocabulary test. Even though an
interaction effect was not found, the gains in terms of the use of imagery and
association strategies for the control group were also statistically
significant. A possible interpretation of this result is that the course
content, i.e. the TOEIC preparation material, may have affected the use of
these strategies.
2 Were there any differences within the experimental group that were based on the learners’ initial repertoire of VLSs?
In order to answer this question, we compared the mean
scores of the pretest and posttest among the clusters, with the Bonferroni
adjustment (p < .05). It should be
noted that the number of participants in Cluster 2 (n = 14) was rather small; therefore, there was a possibility that
it would violate the assumption of parametric tests. For this reason, a
non-parametric test, i.e. the Wilcoxon signed-rank test, was conducted to
double-check the results. This procedure yielded the same results.
Figure 2 Visual representation of the interaction effects of the two dependent
variables. Upper figure: Input-seeking. Lower figure: Oral rehearsal
Figure 3 and
Table 5 present the results of this analysis. Of the three clusters, the
learners in Cluster 1 demonstrated the greatest improvement. In particular,
they significantly gained in Input-seeking (Mdiff = 0.63), Oral
rehearsal (Mdiff = 0.70), Association (Mdiff = 0.52), and
Intrinsic motivation (Mdiff = 0.35). The same tendency was observed for
the learners in Cluster 3 (Oral rehearsal: Mdiff = 0.36;
Association: Mdiff = 0.31). An interesting difference between
Clusters 1 and 3 was the difference in their Intrinsic Motivation scores.
Specifically, Intrinsic motivation increased for the learners in Cluster 1, but
not for the learners in Cluster 3. The fact that Intrinsic Motivation was
enhanced for the learners in Cluster 1 might suggest a possibility that their
motivation increased due to the instruction of VLSs. This result is in line
with previous studies that reported that strategy instruction improves
learners’ motivation (e.g. Nunan, 1997).
Variables
Figure 3 Visual representation of each cluster
Notes : The circles in the upper figure demonstrate
that the difference is significant at p
< .05. Refer to Table 5 for each variable.
In general,
the gains that Cluster 1 exhibited are in line with the findings obtained from
the comparison between the experimental and control groups, i.e. the
experimental group demonstrated greater increases in the use of the
inputseeking and oral rehearsal strategies. In contrast, the scores for Cluster
2 did not exhibit any increase in any of the variables, with the exception of
the vocabulary test (Mdiff = 3.64). Interestingly, this group of
learners marked the largest gains with respect to the vocabulary test.
In sum, the results suggest that the
learners who were less frequent strategy users (Cluster 1) and moderate
strategy users (Cluster 3) benefited the most from the VLSs instruction,
whereas the active strategy users (Cluster 2) did not show any gains in terms
of VLSs use and motivation. The following are noteworthy findings that require
further investigation:
•
the improvements of the learners in Clusters 1 and 3 in
their use of inputseeking, oral rehearsal, or association strategies;
•
the absence of an increase in the use of other
strategies by the same learners;
•
the increase in Intrinsic motivation of the learners in
Cluster 1 before and after the intervention; and
•
why the absence of an increase in the use of VLSs by
the learners in Cluster 2.
These questions are discussed
further in the following section along with the results of the qualitative
analyses of the study logs and interview sessions.
3 What are the reasons behind these differences?
An examination of the study logs and interviews of the
learners in Clusters 1 (less frequent strategy users) and 3 (moderate strategy
users) revealed two reasons for the increased use of input-seeking, oral
rehearsal, or association strategies. The qualitative analysis demonstrated
that they ‘realized the effectiveness of the existing repertoire of strategies’
and were ‘trying the strategies that they thought are useful for them’.
Through strategy instruction, the
learners became more aware of the effectiveness of the strategies they were
already using. One learner provided the following comment at the interview (all
the following excerpts were translated by the authors):
[ Excerpt 1: T3NY ]
When I
memorize words, I always read them aloud as I write them. I have been using
this way of learning since I was a junior high school student. Even when other
people tell me about other strategies, somehow I always keep using this one. I
think this suits my learning style. After the instruction, I once again
realized the effectiveness of vocalizing the words.
Most of the other interviewees
made similar comments. The idea that the VLSs instruction increased awareness
toward the strategies that the learners were already using may also hold true
with respect to input-seeking strategies. In fact, many learners expressed this
view, as seen in the following excerpt:
[ Excerpt 2: T4NR ]
During the course period, I tried
to expose myself to the English language as much as possible because this was
emphasized during the instruction. I watched English TV programs and movies on
DVDs with English subtitles again and again.
Simultaneously, the learners also gained
awareness of the usefulness of the strategies that they had not been using
before the strategy instruction. They understood the usefulness and importance
of the strategies that they were taught and attempted to employ them. This was
particularly effective with respect to association strategies. The following is
a remark that was echoed in many other study log entries:
[ Excerpt 3: T2SY ]
I had never used strategies such as
associating the target words with familiar synonyms or antonyms, using prefixes
and suffixes, and grouping semantically-related words before. I used to try to
remember the meaning of a word through one-to-one correspondence, namely,
between the English and Japanese words, by just repeatedly writing them on a
piece of paper. After I learned the newly introduced strategies, I was able to
feel that there are several ways to make the process of vocabulary learning
easier.
While the VLSs instruction increased
the use of the strategies described above, the same is not true for the other
strategies. The reason for this is that either the learners were already using
them (writing rehearsal strategies) or that some of the strategies were
difficult for them to use even though they seemed useful when they were taught
(imagery strategies). Most of the learners stated in the interview sessions and
also wrote in the study logs that they had encountered difficulties while
attempting to put imagery strategies such as the keyword method into practice.
Initially, they had held the impression that imagery strategies were promising
for facilitating their vocabulary learning. However, they subsequently realized
that it would not be worth the effort considering the amount of time required
to use them. This cost-effectiveness trade-off was echoed in the interviews and
study logs as follows:
[ Excerpt 4: T3MS ]
I cannot
think of a mental image or mnemonics for the target vocabulary. I’m bad at
making them by myself. Also, imagery or mnemonics are not suitable for all the
words. I’d rather spend my time on writing or vocalizing the target words.
The reasons for the increase in
intrinsic motivation for the learners in Cluster 1 may not be easily explained
because a language course involves numerous variables. With this limitation in
mind, we provide the following comments made by two learners in Cluster 1:
[ Excerpt 5: W2OS ]
Since I felt I was able to remember
vocabulary more easily with all the strategies taught, now I feel I can be
better at learning English. The teachers should have taught them to me when I
first started learning English in junior high school.
[ Excerpt 6: W3IK ]
At the moment, I’m not studying English so seriously, but now
that I’ve learned effective ways, I think I can learn more vocabulary when it
becomes necessary. If I can learn vocabulary more easily, I might begin to like
English as a result.
These comments suggest that the instruction of VLSs more or
less contributed to the increase in intrinsic motivation. However, this
increase was observed only for the learners in Cluster 1, namely, the less
frequent strategy users. From this phenomenon stems the following rationale for
incorporating VLSs instruction in regular classroom teaching: Strategy
instruction may have not only improved the use of some strategies, but also
enhanced the learners’ motivation (Nunan, 1997; Cohen and Dörnyei, 2002).
The learners in Cluster 2 (active
strategy users) did not exhibit any increase in their VLSs use. The analyses of
the study logs and interview sessions revealed that these learners seemed to
have already established their own methods for learning vocabulary, and thus,
the instruction did not bring about any changes. To some extent, their use of
VLSs may have already reached saturation point before the intervention was
conducted. This phenomenon was reflected in one interviewee’s comment as
follows:
[ Excerpt 7: T4TS ]
I felt surprise at each lesson
because the strategies introduced were the ones I had already been using. In
fact, it was reassuring to see the strategies being introduced as ‘effective’
approaches to learning vocabulary. Sometimes I felt that I was not using some
of the strategies very efficiently, so it was a good opportunity to review
them.
When the learners in Cluster 2
examined their actual use of strategies at the time of the interviews, they
generally reported more frequent use of strategies than did the learners in
Clusters 1 and 3. This active use of VLSs may account for the former exhibiting
the largest increase in their vocabulary test scores. In this regard, strategy
instruction was useful for directing them in using the VLSs more effectively.
Furthermore, in general, their reaction to the strategy instruction was
positive. Accordingly, it can be suggested that strategy instruction is as
useful for such learners as it is for those whose scores did not change after
the intervention. In fact, this can be the answer to the main concern about
allocating class time for strategy instruction, which is best summarized by
Dörnyei (2005) as follows: ‘It is not clear whether the benefits of their
explicit employment warrant the time and effort spent on them in comparison to
spending the same amount of creative energy designing ‘ordinary’ learning
activities’ (p. 176).
IV Conclusions and implications
In this study, we explored the
effectiveness of explicit instruction of VLSs with Japanese EFL learners. With
regard to the results of the current study, it may well be concluded that the
current study demonstrates the effectiveness of explicit VLSs instruction in
combination with regular classroom instruction. The results can be summarized
as follows:
1)
Explicit teaching of VLSs results in improved
vocabulary test scores.
2)
Explicit teaching of VLSs results in increases of
strategy use among learners with lower and moderate levels of such use.
3)
Explicit teaching of VLSs may result in little change
among learners with high levels of use; however, their teaching can confirm
already held beliefs about their effectiveness.
4)
Some VLSs are quickly rejected due to their
time-consuming nature or being perceived as inefficient in other ways.
5)
Explicit teaching of VLSs may result in more
intrinsically motivated learners.
These results corroborate past findings related to strategy
instruction, which reported that it led to greater strategy use, higher
self-efficacy, increased motivation, wider strategy knowledge, and more
positive attitudes (Chamot et al.,
1996; Nunan, 1997). Moreover, the current study proves that strategy
instruction is more beneficial to less effective learners (Wenden, 1986). On
these grounds, it can be argued that the instruction of VLSs should be further
employed and expanded in normal classroom settings.
Although this
study’s findings contribute to a better understanding of VLSs instruction, a
few limitations should be taken into consideration. First, this study spanned
only one semester (four months), whereas a more longitudinal study could have
produced more extensive results. Second, the proficiency levels of the
participants were rather homogeneous (mostly average or lower-level EFL
learners in Japan), and they were all females. Since proficiency and gender
have been reported to influence the choice and use of strategies (e.g. Oxford
and Nyikos, 1989; Gu and Johnson, 1996), the results of this study might have
been slightly different had we included more proficient learners and male
participants in the research design. Third, although we incorporated in-depth
qualitative analyses, the results of the current study were mainly based on
self-reported questionnaires, i.e. pre–post gains.
Therefore, we might not have accurately captured how well
the learners actually deploy and employ
strategies.
Future research projects should be
directed at improving the quality of teaching various methodologies. In
particular, now that the research on learning strategies has become rather
mature (Cohen and Macaro, 2007), we are better equipped with theoretical and empirical
research findings that can help us construct concrete examples of better
strategy instruction, such as the Styles and Strategies-Based Instruction
(SSBI) developed by Cohen and Weaver (2005). Through the collective efforts of
our colleagues in this field, we will be able to formulate better instruction
models of VLSs for the purpose of developing more autonomous, self-regulated
learners.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to
thank the two anonymous Language Teaching
Research reviewers for their invaluable comments and advice on the earlier
version of the manuscript. An earlier version of this paper was presented at
the Korea TESOL (KOTESOL) International Conference in October 2007, at
Sookmyung Women’s University, Seoul, South Korea.
Notes
1
The TOEIC IP consists of a listening section (100
items) and reading section (100 items). The maximum score for each section is
495 and that for the entire test is 990. According to Educational Testing
Service (2006), ‘TOEIC has been used to measure the English proficiency of
non-native English-speaking people.’
2
T he number of participants from whom TOEIC scores
were obtained was smaller than the actual number of the participants in the
study (n = 146). This is because we
only took into consideration the scores of those who had taken this test in the
last one-year period.
3
I n the taxonomy of Noels et al. (2000), the three items for measuring extrinsic motivation
used in the current study are collectively termed as ‘External Regulation,’ and
the other six items for measuring intrinsic motivation are categorized as
either ‘Intrinsic Motivation – Knowledge’ or ‘Intrinsic Motivation –
Accomplishment.’
4
F or a detailed description of the fit index, see
Tseng et al. (2006).
5
The superiority of this model over other models in the
Japanese educational setting is discussed by the JACET Learning Strategy
Special Interest Group (2005).
V References
Ahmed, M.O.
(1989). Vocabulary learning strategies.
London: British Association for Applied Linguistics in association with Centre
for Information on Language Teaching and Research.
Aizawa, K.
(1998). Developing a vocabulary size test for Japanese EFL Learners. ARELE, 9, 75–82.
Atay, D. and
Ozbuigan, C. (2007). Memory strategy instruction, contextual learning and ESP
vocabulary recall. English for Specific
Purposes, 26, 39–51.
Brown, T.
and Perry, F. (1991). A comparison of three learning strategies for ESL
vocabulary acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 25, 655–70.
Catalán, R.
(2003). Sex differences in L2 vocabulary learning strategies. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 13, 54–77.
Chamot,
A.U., Barnhardt, S., El-Dinary, P.B. and Robbins, J. (1996). Methods for
teaching learning strategies in the foreign language classroom. In Oxford, R.
(Ed.), Language learning strategies
around the world: cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 175–87). Honolulu, HI:
University of Hawaii Press.
Chamot,
A.U., Barnhardt, S., El-Dinary, P.B. and Robbins, J. (1999). The learning strategies handbook. New
York: Longman.
Cohen, A.D.
(1998). Strategies in learning and using
a second language. London: Longman.
Cohen, A.D.
and Aphek, E. (1981). Easifying second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 3, 221–36.
Cohen, A.D.
and Dörnyei, Z. (2002). Focus on the language learner: Motivation, styles, and
strategies. In Schmitt, N. (Ed.), An
introduction to applied linguistics (pp. 170–90). London: Arnold.
Cohen, A.D.
and Macaro, E. (2007). Language learning
strategies: thirty years of research and practice. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Cohen, A.D.
and Weaver, S.J. (2005). Styles and
strategies-based instruction:
a teachers’ guide. Minneapolis, MN: Center for Advanced Research on
Language Acquisition, University of Minnesota.
Craik,
F.I.M. and Lockhart, R.S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory
research. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–84.
Denzin, N.K.
(1997). Triangulation in educational research. In Keeves, J.P. (Ed.), Educational research, methodology and
measurement: an international handbook (pp. 318–22). Oxford: Pergamon.
Dörnyei, Z.
(2005). The psychology of the language
learner: individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dörnyei, Z.
(2007). Research methods in applied
linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Educational
Testing Service (2006). Test of English
for International Communication (TOEIC). Available online at
http://www.ets.org/toeic (August 2009).
Fan, M.
(2003). Frequency of use, perceived usefulness, and actual usefulness of second
language vocabulary strategies: a study of Hong Kong learners. Modern Language Journal, 87, 222–41.
Folse, K.S.
(2004). Vocabulary myths: applying second
language research to classroom teaching. Ann Arbor, MI: University of
Michigan Press.
Gu, Y.
(2003a). Fine brush and freehand: the vocabulary-learning art of two successful
Chinese EFL learners. TESOL Quarterly, 37, 73–104.
Gu, Y.
(2003b). Vocabulary learning in a second language: person, task, context and
strategies. Teaching English as a Second
Language Electronic Journal, 7(2).
Available online at http://www-writing.berkeley.edu/TESL-EJ/ej26/a4.html (
August 2009).
Gu, Y. and
Johnson, R.K. (1996). Vocabulary learning strategies and language learning outcome. Language Learning, 46,
643–79.
Hiromori, T.
(2006). The effects of educational intervention on L2 learners’ motivational
development. JACET Bulletin, 43, 1–14.
Ikeda, M.
(2007). EFL reading strategies: empirical
studies and an instructional model. Tokyo: Shohakusha.
JACET
(2003). JACET8000: JACET word list of 8000 basic words.
Tokyo: JACET.
JACET
Learning Strategy Special Interest Group (2005). Gengo gakushu to gakushu strategy [Language learning and language learning strategies]. Tokyo: Liber
Press.
Kojic-Sabo,
I. and Lightbown, P.M. (1999). Students’ approaches to vocabulary learning and
their relationship to success. Modern
Language Journal, 83, 176–92.
Lawson, J.M.
and Hogben, D. (1996). The vocabulary learning strategies of foreign-language students. Language Learning, 46, 101–35.
Macaro, E.
(2001). Learning strategies in foreign
and second language classrooms. London: Continuum.
McDonough,
S.H. (1999). Learner strategies: state of the art article. Language Teaching, 32,
1–18.
Meara, P.
(1980). Vocabulary acquisition: a neglected aspect of language learning.
Language Teaching and Linguistics Abstracts, 13, 221–46.
Mizumoto, A.
and Shimamoto, T. (2008). A comparison of aural and written vocabulary size of
Japanese university EFL learners. Language
Education and Technology, 45,
35–52.
Mizumoto, A.
and Takeuchi, O. (2008). Exploring the driving forces behind TOEIC scores:
Focusing on vocabulary learning strategies, motivation, and study time. JACET Journal, 46, 17–32.
Mizumoto, A.
and Takeuchi, O. (2009). A closer look at
the relationship between vocabulary learning strategies and the TOEIC scores.
TOEIC Research Report Number 4. Tokyo: The Institute for International Business
Communication.
Nakamura, T.
(2002). Vocabulary learning strategies:
the case of Japanese learners of English. Kyoto: KoyoShobo.
Nation, P.
(1990). Teaching and learning vocabulary.
New York: Newbury House / Harper Row.
Noels, K.A.,
Pelletier, L.G., Clément, R. and Vallerand, R.J. (2000). Why are you learning a
second language? Motivational orientations and self-determination theory. Language Learning, 51, 57–85.
Nunan, D.
(1997). Strategy training in the classroom: an empirical investigation. RELC Journal, 28, 56–81.
Nyikos, M.
and Fan, M. (2007). A review of vocabulary learning strategies: Focus on
language proficiency and learner voice. In Cohen, A. and Macaro, E. (Eds.), Language learning strategies: thirty years
of research and practice ( pp. 251–74). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
O’Malley,
J.M. and Chamot, A.U. (1990). Learning
strategies in second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
O’Malley,
J.M., Chamot, A.U., Stewner-Manzanares, G., Russo, R. and Küpper, L. (1985).
Learning strategy applications with students of English as a second language. TESOL Quarterly, 19, 285–96.
Oxford, R.L.
(1990). Language learning strategies:
what every teacher should know. New
York: Newbury House.
Oxford, R.L.
and Nyikos, M. (1989). Variables affecting choice of language learning
strategies by university students. Modern
Language Journal, 73, 291–300.
Rasekh, Z.E.
and Ranjbary, R. (2003). Metacognitive strategy training for vocabulary
learning. Teaching English as a Second
Language Electronic Journal, 7(2).
Available online at http://www-writing.berkeley.edu/tesl-ej/ej26/a5.html (
August 2009).
Rubin, J.
(1975). What the ‘good language learner’ can teach us. TESOL Quarterly,
9, 41–51.
Rubin, J.,
Chamot, A., Harris, V. and Anderson, N. (2007). Intervening the use of
strategies. In Cohen, A.D. and Macaro, E. (Eds.), Language learning strategies: thirty years of research and practice (pp.
141–60). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sanaoui, R.
(1995). Adult learners’ approaches to learning vocabulary in second languages. Modern Language Journal, 79, 15–28.
Schmitt, N.
(1997). Vocabulary learning strategies. In Schmitt, N. and McCarthy, M. (Eds.),
Vocabulary: description, acquisition and
pedagogy (pp. 199–227). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tabachnick,
B.G. and Fidell, L.S. (2006). Using
multivariate statistics. 5th international edition. Boston, MA:
Pearson/Allyn and Bacon.
Takeuchi, O.
(2003). What can we learn from good language learners?: A qualitative study in
the Japanese foreign language context. System,
31, 385–92.
Takeuchi,
O., Griffiths, C. and Coyle, D. (2007). Applying strategies to context: the
role of individual, situational, and group differences. In Cohen, A. and
Macaro, E. (Eds.), Language learning
strategies: thirty years of research and practice ( pp. 69–92). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
TOEIC
Steering Committee (2006). TOEIC Test
2005 data and analysis. Available online at
http://www.toeic.or.jp/toeic/data/pdf/DAA2005.pdf (August 2009).
Tseng, W.T.,
Dörnyei, Z. and Schmitt, N. (2006). A new approach to assessing strategic
learning: the case of self-regulation in vocabulary acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 27, 78–102. van Lier, L. (2004). The ecology and semiotics of language
learning. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic.
Weinfurt,
K.P. (1995). Multivariate analysis of variance. In Grimm, L.G. and Yarnold,
P.R. (Eds.), Reading and understanding
multivariate statistics. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Wenden, A.
(1986). What do second language learners know about their language learning? A
second look at retrospective accounts. Applied
Linguistics, 7, 186–201.
Wenden, A.
(1991). Learner strategies for learner
autonomy. New York: Prentice Hall.
Yamamori,
K., Isoda, T., Hiromori, T. and Oxford, R. (2003). Using cluster analysis to
uncover L2 learner differences in strategy use, will to learn, and achievement
over time. International Review of
Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 41, 381–409.
Zaki, H. and
Ellis, R. (1999). Learning vocabulary through interacting with a written text.
In Ellis, R. (Ed.), Learning a second
language through interaction ( pp. 153–69). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Zareva, A.,
Schwanenflugel, P. and Nikolova, Y. (2005). Relationship between lexical
competence and language proficiency: variable sensitivity. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 567–95.
Appendix 1: VLSs questionnaire for Japanese EFL learners
( originally in Japanese )
Self-management
1.
I regularly review the vocabulary I learned to check if
I remember it.
2.
I keep a
vocabulary book or word list to check the vocabulary anytime I wish.
3.
I try to make it
a rule to memorize a certain number of words in a specific time period (e.g. ‘I
will memorize 10 words a day’).
4.
I try to learn extra vocabulary in addition to what I
am taught in class.
5.
I try to take time for vocabulary learning.
6.
I consciously set aside time to study vocabulary in
order to prepare for tests (such as TOEIC, TOEFL, or Eiken: English Proficiency
Test).
7.
I use my own
methods for remembering, checking, or reviewing vocabulary.
Input-seeking
8.
I try to expose myself to English vocabulary by reading
or listening a lot.
9.
I try to manage
the learning environment so as to expose myself to English vocabulary.
10. I
try to make use of the media (TV, radio, Internet, mobile phone, or movies) to
learn vocabulary.
11.
I study vocabulary with the intention of using it.
Imagery
12. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I make a mental picture of what can be associated
with a word’s meaning.
13. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I link my personal experiences to it.
14. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I create an image of the spellings or
orthographic forms.
15. W
hen I try to remember vocabulary, I use the keyword method ( keyword mnemonic
technique ).
16.
W hen I try to remember vocabulary, I imagine whether
the meaning of the word is negative or positive.
Writing Rehearsal
17. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I write it repeatedly.
18. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I write it on a note or a card.
19.
W hen I try to remember vocabulary, I remember not only
the meaning but also the spelling of the word by writing it.
Oral Rehearsal
20. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I say it aloud repeatedly.
21. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I vocalize it to remember not only the meaning
but also the pronunciation of the word.
22.
W hen I try to remember vocabulary, I say the sample
sentence aloud.
Association
23. When
I try to remember vocabulary, I associate it with the synonyms (e.g. begin and
start) or antonyms (e.g. positive and negative) I already know.
24. W
hen I try to remember vocabulary, I also memorize the synonyms or antonyms of
the word.
25.
When I try to remember vocabulary, I memorize words
similar to it (in meaning, sound, or shape) or the related words in a group.
Appendix 2: Motivation questionnaire (originally in Japanese)
I study English…
Extrinsic Motivation
1.
B ecause I require school credits to graduate.
2.
I n order to get a prestigious job in the future.
3.
I n order to receive a better salary later on.
Intrinsic Motivation
4.
F or the pleasure I experience when I surpass myself in
my English studies.
5.
F or the enjoyment I experience when I can grasp the
meaning of words if I keep studying.
6.
F or the satisfaction I feel when I am in the process
of accomplishing difficult exercises in English.
7.
F or the ‘high’ I feel when hearing English spoken.
8.
F or the ‘high’ feeling that I experience while
speaking English.
9.
F or the pleasure I get from hearing English spoken by
native speakers of English.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar